Crowd psychology (or mob psychology) is a subfield of social psychology which examines how the psychology of a group of people differs from the psychology of any one person within the group. The study of crowd psychology looks into the actions and Intellectualism of both the individual members of the crowd and of the crowd as a collective social entity. The behavior of a crowd is much influenced by deindividuation (seen as a person's loss of responsibility ) and by the person's impression of the universality of behavior, both of which conditions increase in magnitude with size of the crowd. Notable theorists in crowd psychology include Gustave Le Bon (1841-1931), Gabriel Tarde (1843-1904), and Sigmund Freud (1856-1939). Many of these theories are today tested or used to simulate crowd behaviors in normal or emergency situations. One of the main focuses in these simulation works aims to prevent crowd crushes and stampedes.
Earlier, literature on crowds and crowd behavior had appeared as early as 1841, with the publication of Charles Mackay's book Extraordinary Popular Delusions and the Madness of Crowds. The attitude towards crowds underwent an adjustment with the publication of Hippolyte Taine's six-volume The Origins of Contemporary France (1875). In particular Taine's work helped to change the opinions of his contemporaries on the actions taken by the crowds during the 1789 Revolution. Many Europeans held him in great esteem. While it is difficult to directly link his works to crowd behavior, it may be said that his thoughts stimulated further study of crowd behavior. However, it was not until the latter half of the 19th century that scientific interest in the field gained momentum. French physician and anthropologist Gustave Le Bon became its most-influential theorist.Nye, R. A. (1975). The origins of crowd psychology. London: Sage.Barrows, Susanna (1981). "Distorting mirrors – Visions of the crowd". New Haven: Yale University Press.Van Ginneken, Jaap (1992). Crowds, psychology and politics 1871–1899. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Another approach to classifying crowds is sociologist Herbert Blumer's system of emotional intensity. He distinguishes four types of crowds: casual, conventional, expressive, and active. A group of people who just so happen to be at the same location at the same time is known as a casual crowd. This kind of mob lacks any true identity, long-term goal, or shared connection. A group of individuals who come together for a particular reason is known as a conventional crowd. They could be going to a theater, concert, movie, or lecture. According to Erich Goode, conventional crowds behave in a very conventional and hence somewhat structured manner; as their name suggests, they do not truly act out collective behavior. A group of people who come together solely to show their excitement and feelings is known as an expressive crowd. A political candidate's rally, a religious revival, and celebrations like Mardi Gras are a few examples. An active crowd behaves violently or in other damaging ways, such looting, going above and beyond an expressive crowd. One of the main examples of an acting crowd is a mob, which is an extremely emotional group that either commits or is prepared to do violence. A crowd changes its level of emotional intensity over time, and therefore, can be classed in any one of the four types.
Generally, researchers in crowd psychology have focused on the negative aspects of crowds, but not all crowds are volatile or negative in nature. For example, in the beginning of the socialist movement crowds were asked to put on their Sunday dress and march silently down the street. A more-modern example involves the sit-ins during the Civil Rights movement. Crowds can reflect and challenge the held ideologies of their sociocultural environment. They can also serve integrative social functions, creating temporary communities.
Crowds can be defined as active ("mobs") or passive ("audiences"). Active crowds can be further divided into aggressive, escapist, acquisitive, or expressive mobs. Aggressive mobs are often violent and outwardly focused. Examples are football riots, the Los Angeles riots of 1992, and the 2011 English riots. Escapist mobs are characterized by a large number of people trying to get out of a dangerous situation like the November 2021 Astroworld Festival. Incidents involving crowds are often reported by media as the results of "panic", but some experts have criticized the media's implication that panic is a main cause of crowd disasters, noting that actual panic is relatively rare in fire situations, and that the major factors in dangerous crowd incidents are infrastructure design, crowd density and breakdowns in communication. Acquisitive mobs occur when large numbers of people are fighting for limited resources. An expressive mob is any other large group of people gathering for an active purpose. Civil disobedience, rock concerts, and religious revivals all fall under this category.
Le Bon's idea that crowds foster anonymity and generate emotion has been contested by some critics. Clark McPhail points out studies which show that "the madding crowd" does not take on a life of its own, apart from the thoughts and intentions of members. Norris Johnson, after investigating a panic at a 1979 The Who concert concluded that the crowd was composed of many small groups of people mostly trying to help each other. Additionally, Le Bon's theory ignores the socio-cultural context of the crowd, which some theorists argue can disempower social change. R. Brown disputes the assumption that crowds are homogenous, suggesting instead that participants exist on a continuum, differing in their ability to deviate from social norms.
Theodor Adorno criticized the belief in a spontaneity of the masses: according to him, the masses were an artificial product of "administrated" modern life. The Ego of the bourgeois subject dissolved itself, giving way to the Id and the "de-psychologized" subject. Furthermore, Adorno stated the bond linking the masses to the leader through the spectacle is feigned:
American social psychologist Leon Festinger and colleagues first elaborated the concept of deindividuation in 1952. It was further refined by American psychologist Philip Zimbardo, who detailed why mental input and output became blurred by such factors as anonymity, lack of social constraints, and sensory overload.Zimbardo, Philip (1969). "The human choice – Individuation, reason and order versus Deindividuation, impulse and chaos". Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Vol. 17, pp. 237–307. Zimbardo's Stanford Prison Experiment has been presented as a strong argument for the power of deindividuation, although it was later criticised as unscientific. Further experimentation has had mixed results when it comes to aggressive behaviors, and has instead shown that the normative expectations surrounding the situations of deindividuation influence behavior (i.e. if one is deindividuated as a KKK member, aggression increases, but if it is as a nurse, aggression does not increase).
A further distinction has been proposed between public and private deindividuation. When private aspects of self are weakened, one becomes more subject to crowd impulses, but not necessarily in a negative way. It is when one no longer attends to the public reaction and judgement of individual behavior that antisocial behavior is elicited. Philip Zimbardo also did not view deindividuation exclusively as a group phenomenon, and applied the concept to suicide, murder, and interpersonal hostility.
Convergence theory claims that crowd behavior is not irrational; rather, people in crowds express existing beliefs and values so that the mob reaction is the rational product of widespread popular feeling. However, this theory is questioned by certain research which found that people involved in the 1970s riots were less likely than nonparticipant peers to have previous convictions.
Critics of this theory report that it still excludes the social determination of self and action, in that it argues that all actions of the crowd are born from the individuals' intents.
Key members are identified through distinctive personalities or behaviors. These garner attention, and the lack of negative response elicited from the crowd as a whole stands as tacit agreement to their legitimacy. The followers form the majority of the mob, as people tend to be creatures of conformity who are heavily influenced by the opinions of others. This has been shown in the conformity studies conducted by Muzafer Sherif and Asch. Crowd members are further convinced by the universality phenomenon, described by Allport as the persuasive tendency of the idea that if everyone in the mob is acting in such-and-such a way, then it cannot be wrong.
Emergent norm theory allows for both positive and negative mob types, as the distinctive characteristics and behaviors of key figures can be positive or negative in nature. An antisocial leader can incite violent action, but an influential voice of non-violence in a crowd can lead to a mass sit-in. When a crowd described as above targets an individual, anti-social behaviors may emerge within its members.
A major criticism of this theory is that the formation and following of new norms indicates a level of self-awareness that is often missing in the individuals in crowds (as evidenced by the study of deindividuation). Another criticism is that the idea of emergent norms fails to take into account the presence of existent sociocultural norms. Additionally, the theory fails to explain why certain suggestions or individuals rise to normative status while others do not.
The main idea behind the social identity approach is that people have different social identities, and each one influences how they behave. This idea was central to Reicher’s (1984, 1987)Reicher S. 1984. The St Pauls riot: an explanation of the limits of crowd action in terms of a social identity model. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 14:1-21.Reicher S. 1987. Crowd behaviour as social action. In Rediscovering the Social Group: A Self-Categorization Theory, ed. JC Turner, MA Hogg, PJ Oakes, SD Reicher, MS Wetherell, pp. 171-202. Blackwell. model of crowd behaviour, developed from his study of the 1980 St Pauls riot. Unlike earlier theories, like Le Bon’s, which claimed people lose their sense of self in a crowd, Reicher argued that people don’t lose who their sense of self. Instead, they shift from thinking as individuals to thinking as part of a group. This means their behaviour is not out of control, but guided by the group’s shared beliefs about what’s appropriate. In the St Pauls riot, the crowd didn’t act randomly, their actions reflected a shared group identity and clear sense of purpose.
Reicher (2012)Reicher S. 2012. Crowd psychology. In The Encyclopedia of Human Behaviour, ed. VS Ramanchandran, (2nd Edition). pp.631-637 Elsevier. unified earlier and newer research on social identity and crowd behaviour through a framework called the "three transformations," which helps explain many key theories and findings in this area.
The first transformation is cognitive. It refers to the shift from seeing oneself as an individual to identifying as part of a group. This shift leads people to adopt the group’s norms, values, and goals as their own. When group identity is salient, individuals tend to view others in terms of group membership, respond more to group-consistent messages, and even experience discomfort more positively when it affirms group identity.
The second transformation is relational. It occurs when people not only identify with a group but also recognize that others present share the same identity. This shared identity leads to greater trust, intimacy, cooperation, and coordination. It also forms the basis for distinguishing between physical crowds people and psychological crowds, where shared identity enables spontaneous collective behavior.
The third transformation is affective. As a result of shared identity and support, people feel empowered to express their group identity in ways they usually cannot. This process, known as collective self-objectification, creates a strong sense of emotional positivity and fulfilment within the crowd experience.
While the three transformations framework focuses on internal crowd dynamics, the social identity approach also emphasizes intergroup relations, particularly between crowds and external groups like the police (Reicher, 1984, 1987). Reicher’s original model evolved into the Elaborated Social Identity Model (ESIM), which explains how conflict in crowd events arises through identity change (Drury & Reicher, 2000; Reicher, 1996; Stott & Reicher, 1998).Drury J, Reicher S. 2000. Collective action and psychological change: The emergence of new social identities. Brit. J. Soc. Psychol. 39:579-604.Reicher S. 1996. ‘The Battle of Westminster’: developing the social identity model of crowd behaviour in order to explain the initiation and development of collective conflict. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 26:115-34.Stott C, Reicher S. 1998. How conflict escalates: the inter-group dynamics of collective football crowd ‘violence’. Sociology. 32:353-77.
According to ESIM, two main conditions contribute to the development and escalation of conflict between a crowd and another group, such as the police. First, there is a clash in how each group categorizes the behaviour at the event. For example, crowd members may see their actions as a legitimate protest, while police may interpret them as a threat to public order. Second, there is an imbalance of power, with the police having the ability to enforce their definition of what behavior is acceptable, often through physical control or dispersal.
When police action is seen by the crowd as illegitimate - such as being perceived as an attack on their right to protest - it can justify resistance. If that action is also viewed as indiscriminate, affecting everyone in the crowd regardless of individual behaviour, it can create a sense of shared fate. This shared experience can lead to the formation of a common identity within an otherwise diverse crowd, overriding internal differences and enhancing.
As this new collective identity forms, it brings a sense of unity, enhanced expectations of support for ingroup-normative actions, and legitimacy for resisting the outgroup (in this case, the police).
The social identity approach to the crowd was extended to explain behaviour in mass emergencies. This was often found to involve cooperation amongst strangers, and research suggested that this spontaneous coordination reflected the emergence in the crisis of a new shared social identity (Drury, 2018).Drury J. 2018. The role of social identity processes in mass emergency behaviour: an integrative review. Eur. Rev. Soc. Psychol. 29(1):38-81. One of the most notable developments in crowd behaviour research over the past 20 years has been the application of the social identity approach to everyday, non-crisis crowd settings, including religious mass gatherings, music events, and pedestrian flow (Drury, 2025).Drury J. 2025.
Theoretical perspectives
Le Bon
Freudian theory
"When the leaders become conscious of mass psychology and take it into their own hands, it ceases to exist in a certain sense. ... Just as little as people believe in the depth of their hearts that the Jews are the devil, do they completely believe in their leader. They do not really identify themselves with him but act this identification, perform their own enthusiasm, and thus participate in their leader's performance. ... It is probably the suspicion of this fictitiousness of their own 'group psychology' which makes fascist crowds so merciless and unapproachable. If they would stop to reason for a second, the whole performance would go to pieces, and they would be left to panic."T. W. Adorno, "Freudian Theory and the Pattern of Fascist Propaganda." In Vol. III of Psychoanalysis and the Social Sciences. Ed. Géza Roheim. New York: International Universities Press, 1951, pp. 408–433. Reprinted in Vol. VIII of Gesammelte Schriften. Frankfurt: Suhrkamp Verlag, 1975, and in The Culture Industry: Selected Essays on Mass Culture. Ed. J. M. Berstein. London: Routledge, 1991.
Deindividuation theory
Convergence theory
Emergent norm theory
Social identity approach
See also
Further reading
External links
|
|